Issue 27. Fall 1996
The Association for the Study of Language In Prehistory (ASLIP) is a nonprofit organization, incorporated under the laws of the Commonwealth of Massachusetts. Its purpose is to encourage and support the study of language in prehistory in all fields and by all means, including research on the early evolution of human language, supporting conferences, setting up a data bank, and publishing a newsletter and a journal to report these activities.
Membership: Annual dues for ASLIP membership and subscription to Mother Tongue are US $25 in all countries, except those with currency problems. For membership information, contact: Harold C. Fleming, Secretary-Treasurer A.S.L.I.P. 16 Butman Avenue Gloucester, MA 01930-1006 USA
OFFICERS OF ASLIP (Address appropriate correspondence to each)
President John D. Bengtson / 1329 Adams Street NE / Minneapolis,
MN 55413, USA / Tel. 612-348-5910
Vice President Daniel McCall / 7 Wigglesworth St / Boston, MA 02114
USA / Tel. 617-277-1434 / 508-627-5571 (summer)
Vice President Roger Williams Wescott / 16-A Heritage Crest / Southbury,
CT 06488 USA / Tel. 203-264-1716
Secretary-Treasurer Harold C. Fleming (see above). Tel. 508-282-0603
BOARD OF DIRECTORS
Ofer Bar-Yosef (Peabody M, Harvard)
Jerold Harmatz (Tufts U.)
Anne W. Beaman (Brookline, MA)
John Hutchison (Boston U.)
Allan R. Bomhard (Charleston, SC)
Judith Leader (Lexington, MA)
Ronald Christensen (Lincoln, MA)
Philip Lieberman (Brown U.)
Frederick Gamst (U / Massachusetts)
COUNCIL OF FELLOWS
Raimo Anttila (UCLA)
Sydney Lamb (Rice University)
Luigi Luca Cavalli-Sforza (Stanford)
Winfred P. Lehmann (U/Texas)
Igor M. Diakonoff (St. Petersburg)
Karl-Heinrich Menges (U/Vienna)
Aaron Dolgopolsky (U/Haifa)
Colin Renfrew (Cambridge U., UK)
Ben Ohiomamhe Elugbe (U/Ibadan)
Vitalij Shevoroshkin (U/Michigan)
Joseph H. Greenberg (Stanford)
Sergei Starostin (Academy of Sciences, Russia)
Carleton T. Hodge (U/Indiana)
Dell Hymes (U/Virginia)
Copyright 1996 Association for the Study of Language in Prehistory
ISSN 1087-0326 for Mother Tongue: The Journal.
INTRODUCTION TO MT-27: The Newsletter
(Editor this issue: H. Fleming)
THE HOTTEST AND THE LATEST NEWS, AS OF MID-NOVEMBER, 1996.
The hottest, latest news is not necessarily the most important news
-- in the wisdom of hindsight it may even be irrelevant to our common enterprise.
But, since the items are new, they have within them the potential of establishing
something or dis-establishing something else. This time around, the hottest
news comes from Australia and Virginia but both badly need some/more confirmation.
Our traditional mode of presentation features fossils first.
DELAYED COMMENTS ON THE BASQUE CRITIQUE + NIHALI IN MT-II, COMING
UP
The father of Sino-Caucasic, Sergei Starostin, has spoken, saying
enough to be put in the Journal (MT-II) rather than the Newsletter. The
editorial board of Bengtson, Wescott, and McCall will soon produce MT-II
whose central focus will be on Nihali of India, with MT*Treat-ment of new
Nihali data and the classification of this most difficult language, so
potentially vital to South Asian prehistory.
NEWS OF MEMBERS' ACTIVITIES, INCLUDING LETTERS OF COMMENT
This rich lode of material has proven too much for MT-27, if we
are to publish before New Years.
We promise MT-28 with this lode by February.
ANNOUNCEMENTS & ADVERTISEMENTS: THE MEMBERSHIP (PERMITTED) LIST.
The eagerly awaited list of members who permit their names to be
listed. While not necessarily an act of singular courage at a time of rampant
conformity, giving permission to publish asserts something, like "This
is no Communist cell. Let them know I belong! And I want to discuss prehistory
with good colleagues!" (Many members plan to face this issue, answer the
questionnaire and pay their dues in their own good time -- later. So remember:
the list of permissions does not equal the whole list of members, even
including some Directors and Fellows. Will half of you please pay your
dues? We cannot afford to mail out Journals to those who hoard money.)
OBITUARIES:
John Kerna, Søren Egerod, Jan Winter, Mary Haas, R. Stopa.
We regret that the presentation of these has been deferred a while longer.
Since we have noted the deaths of these valuable colleagues, we may be
forgiven for 'bumping' their obituaries in order to present the hottest
news first. Said news is very 'time sensitive' as they say nowadays, while
the obituaries lack that attribute.
ASLIP BUSINESS
There is not much. A brief report on the Utah meetings. A small
grant in aid from Boston University. List of libraries is growing. February!
INFORMATIONAL EDITORIAL: BORROWING SOME WISDOM FROM KINDRED SCIENCES.
We find remarkable similarities or analogies in our sister sciences.
Michael Day (paleoanthropology), Lynn Margolis (deep taxonomy biology)
APOLOGIES! I was sick too much, traveled too much, etc. Sorry tardy!
************** ************* ****************
THE HOTTEST AND THE LATEST NEWS, AS OF MID-NOVEMBER,
1996
Amazing New Data from Australia
Other than the vexing uncertainties of Chinese dating and the continuous
resistance of Michigan's Wolpoff, no serious empirical challenge to the
Africa first hypothesis of modern human origins has remain-ed upright.
New excavations in Australia at one swell foop threaten the whole notion
of an African diaspora for Homo sapiens s. Modern humans able to produce
art and artifacts lived in northwest Australia (Jinmium, in the Kimberley
area) a long time before most consensus dates for the African exodus, like
many thousands of years earlier! More exactly, a site with presumed artifacts
dates to 116,000 to 176,000 ya. The art consists of thousands of circles
(all about 3 centimeters across) carved into boulders. But the art is apparently
dated only to 66,500 ya. Or more precisely "a chunk of boulder bearing
the circles lay in a stratum dated to between 58,000 and 75,000 years old."
While that is very old for art, still it doesn't threaten the Africa hypothesis.
We reported on similar Aussie art dates in MT-25.
Key questions which arise are three: (a) were they really moderns?,
(b) was it really art?, and (c) above all, are the dates really true? One
easily imagines a population of late Homo erectus type humans who did crude
stuff which just barely qualifies as art. Since the dating is by thermoluminesence,
a sometimes unsure procedure, then perhaps the 'wild' dates of 116-176
kya are bogus? It has happened before that such dates were off by a lot,
always too old. Yes, but they have also been right! And we do not know
which it is this time -- right or wrong! By any reckoning the new Aussie
material is a major contribution to global pre-history, unless of course
it turns out to be 'bad data'. If the dates are only 50k or 60k too old,
then as corrected they would be nearly contemporaneous with the African
consensus dates of circa 100 kya. If the artists so-called were not Homo
sap sap, they would constitute an advanced Homo erectus occupancy of Australia
which we have known nothing about before this. It would roughly equal the
older discovery in Europe of advanced Homo erectus / archaic Homo sapiens,
i.e., Neanderthal with Mousterian culture. Again these early Australians
would mean significant sea-faring for non-modern humans because Australia
cannot be reached any other way. We do have to assume that there was a
population of sorts, as Henry Harpending would wish, rather than one pregnant
female accidentally floating over on a piece of driftwood. Full reports
of exactly what was excavated and how old various strata are and how well
stratified the site(s) is -- all that is supposed to be coming out in Antiquity
in December.
We are operating off reports from (a) the New York Times (Sept. 22),
(b) Allan Bomhard reporting it from e-mail, and (c) a brief summary in
Science (Oct.4, 33-4) For those impatient to hear more: the authors of
the Antiquity paper are Richard Fullagar of the Australian Museum (Sydney)
+ Lesley Head and David Price, both of the University of Wollongong, New
South Wales, in the School of Geosciences. For the absolutely latest progress
in dating with new OSL methods contact Rhys Jones (Australian National
University, Canberra) or Richard Roberts (La Trobe University, Melbourne).
OSL = optically stimulated luminescence, a slightly different dating technique.
They mean to test the TL dates. David Price says that C14 dates
of 'upper levels' already confirm the TL dates. However, with dates of
this magnitude involved, C14 with its own limitations may weakly support
but cannot confirm much of the deep stuff. The Jinmium site provokes more
thinking. First, one can not help comparing MacNeish's cave in New Mexico
which also had startlingdates and novel artifacts but was doubted to death
by archeologists. The second is the parallel story of New Guinea and proper
Melanesia where we find modern humans in the islands by 38,000 ya. Homo
sap sap didn't get to Europe much earlier but had been on New Guinea maybe
10,000 years before that. So modern humans have lived east of Wallace's
Line longer than most places, even if the Jinmium site fails to live up
to its publicity.
Maginot Line: The Shredding.
The news from Virginia, reported by Mammoth Trumpet, is not good
for advocates of the Clovis Horizon as first Amerind settlement of North
America. Two sites with clear stratigraphy contained levels below
the Clovis Horizon and with dates just too old to be accomodated by Clovis
theory. Just as the Rockefellar team's Amazonian sites could not be crammed
into the Procrustean Bed of accept-able dates for early humans in the Americas.
Americanist arche-ologists now must surround two more sites with the sticky
bubble of their scepticism. Let us pray for them for truly their labors
become too demanding! Or maybe the image of Peter at the Dike would be
more suitable?
What are the sites? In eco-logically distinct areas of Virginia.
The first is Cactus Hill in the tidewater ('Nieder-länder') area of
southeastern Viginia, 45 miles south of Richmond. There were found one
or two cultural levels beneath an established Clovis level. The finds were
"simple stone tools -- blades and cores"; the pre-Clovis level has been
dated to 15,070 BP by AMS procedures (accelerator mass spectrometer) but
the Clovis levels were also dated by C14 (10,920 ya) and by the presence
of white pine charcoal. That tree has not been in the tidewater for 10,000
years, said a Yale paleobotanist (McWheeny). Negative aspects of the site
include sand dunes and the extensive looting of the upper levels or over
400 of 1400 square feet of site or 30% ±.
The other site is Saltville in southwestern Virginia in a valley
of the Appalachian Mts. Here were found "stone and bone tools, mastodon
bones and ivory, fire-cracked rock, and other evidence". While the site
was dated by C14 to 13,990 ± 70 on a "twig in a sand lens that integ-rates
a variety of presumably cultural material", it also had a C13-adjusted
date of 13, 950 ± 70. That is the better part of 14kya. While the
principal investigator was not an archeologist, his credentials sound fairly
good, i.e., research associate in paleo-biology at the Smithsonian Institution's
National Museum of Natural History. His name is Jerry N. McDonald. His
team has been laboring away at the site for fifteen years.
Sources are: MammothTrumpet October, 1996, and the chief of the
Cactus Hill operations, Joseph M.McAvoy, Archeological Society of Virginia,
5861 White Oak Road, Sandston, VA 23150, USA.
That same issue of the 'Strumpet had some wonderfully interesting
cranial/facial reconstructions of a 9900 year old American woman from near
modern Austin, Texas. She has been lovingly named 'Wilson-Leonard II' by
excavation logic but surely we can improve on that, as we did in Lucy's
case. There are two casts, one made by forensic artist Betty P. Gatliff
and the other by Arthur H. Rathjens and colleagues of Dow Corning Corp.
The results are different to my eye, albeit much like sisters. Although
Amerinds have often had a nondescript or unspecialized look, I am hard
put to locate either model in a 'race'. Gatliff's could easily be an ancestor
common to both Caucasoids and Mongoloids, or simply Caucasoid. The other
looks strikingly like a forest tribal person from Southeast Asia, as pictured
in Coon's Living Races of Man, or just as easily a pastoral Cushite from
the Horn of Africa. Remarkable!
Also in the 'Strumpet was the second part of an extended interview
they did with Marta Mirazon Lahr. Since we have already reported much of
her work (in MT-26), I confine myself to pointing out that her dispersal
map of early humans entering the New World has them traveling by a coastal
route, rather than through the Rocky Mountain passes, and via the Japan-Aleutian
route. I point out humbly that we proposed just that several years ago
herein.
Critical Mass: It's Been Reached As this is not nuclear physics,
nothing will explode. Yet we surely have reached a point where it's getting
obvious that too many pre-Clovis sites in North and South America have
been found. Too many, that is, for a stubborn doctrine that nothing before
11,500 ya can stand up to the scrutiny of expert archeologists. (Does this
ever sound like Americanist linguistics!)
Let us rehearse a bit. The following sites with their dates have
been published by competent professional archeologists. All are earlier
than the Clovis level of 11.5kya. Here they are: Saltville (USA) 15,000
Cactus Hill (USA) 14,000 Meadowcroft (USA) 14-17,000 Nebraska (mammoth
site) 18,000 Alberta (Canada) > 20,000 Pendejo Cave (USA) 28,000 ±
Monte Alegre (Brazil) late Pleistocene contemporary Pedra Furada (Brazil)
> 20,000 Monte Verde (Chile) > 13,000 excavator has receded from deeper
levels and dates. Marta Lahr mentioned three sites in far southern South
America with dates close to the Clovis line. Again was there a 'forced
march' migration from North America? Can all of the above be dismissed
as not up to standard? Clovis Culture in Siberia! Presumably the vaunted
Clovis horizon in North America was the cultural appearance of the first
Amerind and/or the first modern humans in the New World. (It seems that
no one believes the intruders went to South America first.) That culture
dates to 11,200 to 8500 years ago (ya) or Before Present (BP) and has been
the bedrock of certitude for American archeo-logists for at least a decade
now.
Presumably said Clovis people arrived from Asia as colonial immigrants,
since Pardner Hicks has not yet persuaded scholars that Asians were emigrants
from America. The search has been going on then, quietly, for the residues
of that Asian or pre-proto-Amerind colonial culture and population in Siberia
or Eastern Asia. Just as final shrugs of regret for not finding anything
relevant were being made -- lo and behold! voila! -- a similar culture
or at least a similar stone industry has been found in the Russian Far
East at Uptar, Magadan Oblast, northeastern Siberia between the Kolyma
upland and the Sea of Okhotsk about 1920 km west of the Bering Straits.
Its firm dates are from a stratum above the Clovis type stratum, 8260 ±
330 ya by C14, so the "Clovis" level at Uptar has to be older, maybe much
older. At a rough guess it seems a virtual contemporary of the North American
Clovis strata.
Let us back track just for a moment because the reporters on Uptar
have a nice summary of their regrets mentioned above. "the earliest firmly
documented tradition in eastern Siberia (the Upper Paleolithic Diuktai
from the Aldan basin, 35,000 to 10,000 years B.P.) .. is thought .. to
bear little resemblance to Paleoindian traditions." Let us go over this
point for non-arch-eologists. Fossil cultures as old as 35,000 ya but not
much like Clovis have been found in the Lena river basin of Siberia. So
it is hard to derive the later emigrants to America from Diuktai culture.
However, Uptar provides the 'marker tool' (my coinage) that shows serious
similarity with Clovis, namely lanceolate bifacial points, especially fluted
points. Most of the 3100 artifacts collected were chipped stone; of them
shaped artifacts are mostly bifaces or fragments of bifaces. Also a few
cores, flake tools, some blades, with some microblades, were found. Yet
the points were not ident-ical to Clovis, being smaller in size and lack
grinding on the edges and base. Nor would they fall exactly within the
Nenana complex of Alaska or other cultures of Siberia or Alaska. The authors
do not believe that Uptar is evidence for a source whence Clovis culture
came, nor for a counter-emigration from North America. In linguistic terms
we could say that there was a typological similarity without genetic links
or evid-ence of borrowing. Or to quote the authors more directly: "The
Uptar site shows that the early prehistory of northeastern Siberia is more
diverse than traditional colonization models imply .. The focus on defining
technologically distinct migrat-ory groups (for example, pre-microblade
versus microblade complexes) may neglect important aspects of assemblage
variabil-ity, especially as they pertain to issues of the peopling of Beringia."
Source: Maureen L. King and Sergei B. Slobodin. 1996. "A Fluted Point from
the Uptar Site, Northeastern Siberia". SCIENCE 273, 2 August, 634-6. She
is at Dept. of Anthropology, U/Washington or Desert Research Institute,
Quarternary Sciences Center, P.O.Box 19040, Las Vegas, Nevada 89132 USA.
He is at Dept. of Education, Apartment 19, 14 Dzerzhinsky Street, Magadan,
68500 Russia.
We also recommend previous issues of Mother Tongue in which many
of these Siberian and Alaskan sites were discussed more thoroughly. We
should also remark that Uptar is entirely compatible with the coastal route
to the Americas which Marta Lahr advocated above and ourselves earlier
on. The same for the Ushki site on Kamchatka, but not for Diuktai. Why
Stress Clovis Horizons? Some may have forgotten why we stress this argument
about the Clovis horizon, since we started it a decade ago and have been
bringing it up again and again.
Several reasons exist: first, for archeologists with very high standards
and strongly empirical orientations it is a quality problem. Brian Fagin
argued eloquently for quality many issues ago in MT. This is the archeological
equivalent of the linguist wanting only the best descriptive grammar before
venturing an opinion. It cannot be faulted in its own terms because: who
would prefer lousy data to the bright and shiny stuff? We can say, however,
that such does not justify ignoring decent or suggestive data, albe-it
possibly defective, that point to inferences different from the tightly-laced
conclus-ions considered to be correct at the time. Weak and wobbly data
or seemingly speculative theory may in fact presage major para-digm shifts
soon to be required of the discipline. There still is no better model for
this than Wegner's drifting continents which so revolutionized geology.
Second, many long rangers find in the Clovis horizon, itself spread
over so much of the New World at the earliest date, a neat counterpart
for the ancestor of the linguistic super-phylum = proto-Amerind. It is
extremely tempting to settle for such a correlation! It warms the scientific
heart to solve puzzles and the Clovis=Amerind solution verily warms some
hearts. Third, three considerations impinge on the above heart-felt theory.
(a) Since Americanist linguists have no hypotheses of much serious time
depth, piously insisting that no relationships as old as 10 kya can be
known at all, their influence does not come to bear on the Clovis question.
(b) It has always seemed to this Africanist that the time depths discernible
in the many obviously divergent Amerind languages could not be contained
within 11 millennia, (the Clovis framework) but had to be much older than
that. Take as one example the 500 Bantu tongues of Africa whose genetic
unity is one of the 'obvious' things in historical linguistics but whose
time depth has at least 3000 years (with strong archeological correlates)
in it. Only a few sectors of Amerind have such an obvious quality as Bantu,
e.g., Iriquoian, Algon-kian, etc, but a brief look at Mexican languages
will quickly dispel the notion of obvious. (c) The dates generated by bio-genetic
theory have always ex-ceeded 10 kya when discussing native American populations.
No one has to remind geneticists that their dates remain unsub-stantiated;
they admit that themselves. But the dates remain scientific hypotheses
which have not been falsified -- yet. So they do bear on questions of Amerind
origins. (For one false calculation see below: 'Cows')
Hot Linguistic News from Africa
Well known are the four increasingly solid linguistic phyla in Africa,
courtesy of the Greenberg classification. What is often not understood
by non-Africanists is the profound diversity within all of the African
phyla. Anyone of them would be extremely controversial if their languages
were spoken within the I-E or Americanist realms. I was amazed at one conference
by a senior Oceanist who pooh poohed Greenberg's methods because the African
languages were so much alike, he believed. In effect anybody could classify
them -- peas in a pod they are. Simple stuff, not like the diversity of
Oceania! How do you explain to an Australian that his country is actually
upside down? It is that kind of problem. Along with that ignorant old Oceanist
there have been several Indo-Europeanists who have taken the same stance.
Some have published as much, although the references do not come to mind
right now. I find such people astonishing in their provincialism. Everyone
has to know something about I-E, since all the text books fashionably focus
on I-E, yet so many of these parochial Aryanologists really know very little
about Africa. Well then, how do we explain to these Australians that their
country is actually upside down? Hmmm.
Might we use a universal measure of congruence versus diversity?
like percentages of basic lexicon or whatever one can think of? One measure
might be how many phyla a master taxonomist (e.g., Jos. Greenberg) could
find in any area. If languages can't be classified together, then they
are surely remote from each other -- at least. In the home of novice taxonomists
-- the Americas -- we can at once infer diversity since the specialists
can find no less than 100 phyla. Indeed in one tour de force they make
South America the most likely source of modern humans; it has no less than
16 times as many phyla as any other region on earth. If Pardner Hicks is
listening, he will rejoice for this is the best evidence he can get for
an American homeland for Homo sap sap.
However, by the criterion we started with above, South America has
one phylum. Of course in an area like India where we find the record for
the Old World -- eight phyla -- still five of them appear to be intrusive
(I-E, Dravidian, Munda, S-T, and Daic) or have very strong kin links on
the outside. So let us count only those which are totally native, cannot
arguably be derived from any outside group. Then India's 3 are matched
by SouthEast Asia & Oceania's 3 and by Africa's 3. That is, Burushaski,
Kusunda, Nihali are India's natives, while Australian, Indo-Pacific and
Austric do for the Pacific Rim. If one does not accept S-T as Sino-Caucasic,
and by our criterion we cannot, then the Rim has 4 phyla and is champ.
Africa loses Afrasian as part of Nostratic, keeping Khoisan, Nilo-Saharan
and Niger-Congo. Again by our criterion, JHG has several times agreed informally
that Afrasian was a member of Nostratic, but maybe coordinate. And in these
terms Europe, of course, has only one -- Basque.
Now the news. Christopher Ehret has proposed
that Africa in reality has six phyla, adding Shabo and Krongo to the list.
As he announced this obscurely four years ago, when I was not tracking
the subject, it never got into MT. Sorry for the delay! But thanks to Paul
White-house (London) for pointing out Ehret's article of 1995.
Shabo is spoken by a small group of hunters inside of Majang society,
itself hunter-farmers. We reported Shabo in earlier issues. Fleming is
on record, postulating Shabo's kinship with Koman inside Nilo-Saharan (N-S).
Krongo is a new name for the group associated with Kadugli, i.e., formerly
a part of Kordofanian of Niger-Congo. Thilo Schadeberg excised that group
from Niger-Congo and put it in N-S instead. Both Fleming's and Schadeberg's
hypotheses were reported in MT. Ehret's new phyla are debatable, even as
he sees them. But his main argument is that both Shabo and Kadugli are
so far from N-S and N-C that they cannot be incorporated in either phylum.
Rather they should be seen as independent phyla, possibly related of course
to N-S or N-C ultimately at a very deep level. It seems to me that we both
agree that Shabo is very remote from anyone, while I see it as belonging
to N-S in a primary branch or moiety with Koman at the root of the tree.
In reality these are very simil-ar views and reflect the great diversity
within N-S or within Africa. In discussing Shabo in 1989 I mentioned the
complete lack of fit between its grammar, here to mean morphology, and
those of neighboring languages. By now it has become apparent that all
field workers have used the same informant and he is unreliable for grammar.
The lexica recorded are not too dissimilar. So we may pray for better field
work.
In the case of Krongo we frankly solicit the opinions of other Africanists
or globalists, since my own expertise in Kordofan is too limited. Chris
Ehret may be right cause he is a fine taxonomist. But Thilo's sharp eyes
caught the problem in the first place, so his may be the right answer.
As far as African diversity is concerned, we rest our case. The implications
for the Greenberg classification need to be specified. They do not prove
the inadequacy of that work as might be thought. Rather the advent of this
new data (Shabo) could not be predicted in 1948-63. Plus the Krongo question
arises from newer field data, more intensive study, and uncertainty. When
the dust settles, we may discover that Greenberg was mistaken about the
total membership of Kordofanian. Or he was not. It's not a big deal, at
least not for a mature science. For absolute truth seekers -- it's a problem.
Sources: Christopher Ehret. 1995. "Do Krongo and Shabo belong in
Nilo-Saharan?". Robert Nicolaï et Franz Rottland, eds., Fifth Nilo-Saharan
Linguistics Colloquium. Nice, 24-29 août 1992. Proceedings, pp.169-193.
Köln, Köppe Verlag. Also Ehret's advanced work on N-S is coming
out soon as a book called Reconstructing Nilo-Saharan. Publisher to be
announced.
Since the Shabo live in the rain forests of southwestern Ethiopia,
speaking a language too remote to be satisfactorily classified, then one
senses that biogeneticists will be just as interested in Shabo bodies as
they are in Pigmies or Bushmen. Thus it is a bit amusing but too sad to
report that a trained biogeneticist walked right by a group of Shabo in
Ethiopia last year, never knowing who they were. Thus reported Peter Unseth
who would have told the fellow!
COWS & GENES: A miscalculation?
We reported biogenetic dates for cattle domestication in MT-26. Well,
the European date of 5000 is false, since domestic cows in Turkey are 1000+
years earlier than that. (Bar-Yosef, p.c.) All this spells concern for
African cattle theories and dates. If anything, judging by the Turkish
test, the African domesticates will turn out to be even older than 9 kya!
Say 11,000 years ago by proportional assumptions. Hmm.
Deep Biogenetic Calculations
Having shown biogenetic errors, we want to highlight some recent
& exciting work done by Stephen Sherry in his doct-oral dissertation
at Penn State (with Mark Stoneking as Thesis Advisor). Steve will present
a fuller version of his conclusions next year in the Journal along with
MT*Treatment. This will feature an English translation from the BioGenetic
Speak dialect of English. Most crucially, the dating involved in Steve's
thesis is more relational than absolute. That is, he produces time periods
during which crucial events of human evolution and prehistory occurred,
rather than a specific absolute date. Since the orientation is both towards
DNA and population genetics, the result is a sort of progression of events
happening to human populations. We present here his formal abstract from
his dissertation, knowing full well that many long rangers will not understand
it, because his thesis is important. We all will extend ourselves to try
to savvy what is said, so that next year the effort will be easier to grasp.
Thus saith his abstract:
"Data on human genetic diversity for two independently evolving
genetic systems, mito-chondrial DNA (mtDNA) and nucle-ar Alu elements,
and mtDNA diversity in common chimpanzees were analyzed by three low dimension
distributions methods which fit models of demographic history to the data
by intensive computer simulation: mismatch distribution analysis, multi-variate
crossmatch and segregat-ing sites analysis, and spectrum of heterozygosity.
Although the three methods use different properties of coalescent theory
to derive their respective probabilities of gene identity and patterns
of genetic diver-gence under demographic flux, they sugest a common hour-glass
shaped pattern of human demographic history. We conclude that hominid numbers
fell from a long-term effective size of 100,000 down to 20,000 by 400,000
years ago, and to a minimum size of approximately 5000 total individuals
by 100,000 years ago before recov-ery at 50,000 years ago. Common chimpanzees
show a similar episode of collapse from 55,000 to 85,000 years ago." (Quote
ends)
Sherry's research has been submitted to several journals for publication.
Preliminary discussion with colleagues has indicated that the locus of
populations referr-ed to may be critical to under-standing and evaluating
Steve's argument. For example, 5000 may be a substitute for Cann's "Eve"
or proto-Mother. See Henry Harp-ending's arguments in MT-19. Or 5000 may
stand for all Homo on earth in 100,000 BP -- in which case we may doubt
the 5000 on empirical grounds. Some think that known populations of Homo
erectus plus archaic Homo s.s. greatly exceeded 5000 in total at that date.
Und so weiter.
Paleolithic Paradigm Rocking On the frontiers.
Tomorrow we may see some of the familiar verities of the Upper Paleolithic
of western Asia, especially the circum-Mediterranean area, somewhat differently.
New archeological data from western Asia indicate that four 'facts' will
be changed. First, that Neanderthal may be earlier there than in Europe.
Second, the Middle Paleolithic is much older than expected, i.e., 250 kya.
Third, that things usually asso-ciated with Cro-Magnon or Auri-gnacian
culture may also be present in Mousterian, e.g., art (minimally figurines)
and blades. Fourth, that the Upper Paleolithic was very slow to spread
from either the Levant or Europe to the Caucasus or even the interior of
Syria. Slow = 35 kya, not 40 kya. Yet Aurignacian persisted in Greece until
20 kya which may be the source of the same in Israel in 18 kya. All of
this frontier stuff awaits the publication of sites now being excavated.
Source: Ofer Bar-Yosef's extensive survey of sites this year and
personal communication to me. He is not responsible for any errors I made
in reporting or interpreting his communication.
Homo Gets Older in Ethiopia
Absolutely the latest news, reported in Boston Globe and NY Times
Nov. 19th before its publication in the Journal of Human Evolution in December.
For our purposes it is not the hottest kind of news, however.
The jaw of a hominid, called the first of genus Homo by taxonomists,
was found in Hadar of the Dana-kil lowlands of northeast Ethiopia (Afar)
by an international team of scientists. Being an upper jaw, it had inferential
value for the face which was broader than Australopithecus afarensis with
a parabolic dental arch, a short flat nose and no "projecting, apelike"
face, i.e. no prognathism. The dates of 2.33 mya are about a half million
years earlier than other Homo genus forms, making this the oldest known
form of that genus and by implication the oldest evidence of the start
of our human line. In a sense the old 'missing link' which Haile Selassie
feared. Alleged stone tools came with the jaw, but remember MacNeish's
troubles with geofacts! Fossils pour in now: an area 5 times as big as
Texas is being searched. Tribute to Berkeley's team: W.H.Kimball, R.C.
Walter, and D.C. Johanson.
Some Members of Association for the Study of Language In Prehistory
Introductory Note: The following list is not a true list of the members of ASLIP for two reasons. First, some did not want their names listed (for all kinds of reasons). Second, barely 35% of the members returned the questionnaires, so 65% never said whether they would permit their names to be published or not. This even includes a number of Board members and Council Fellows, as well as some very well known long rangers. Consequently, the list is a true list only of ASLIPers who returned the questionnaire and allowed their names to be published. The label USA is omitted for American members.
Members and their Addresses: Those Who Permit the Publication
David W. Anthony :: Dep't of Anthropology, Hartwick College, Oneonta,
NY 13820
Yoël Arbeitman :: Institute of Semitic Studies, 195 Nassau
Street, Princeton, NJ 08542
Brigitte M. Bauer :: Staringstraat 34, 6521 AK Nijmegen, The
Netherlands.
Anne Windsor Beaman :: P. O. Box 583, Brookline, Mass. 02146-0005
Wolfgang Behr :: Johann Wolfgang Goethe-Universität, Sinologie,
Postfach 111 932, 60054 Frankfurt a.M., Germany.
Paul Benedict :: 104 River Lane, Ormond Beach, FL 32176
John D. Bengtson :: 1329 Adams Street NE, Minneapolis, MN 55413.
(Tel. 612-348-5910 in USA)
Robert Blust :: Dep't. of Linguistics, Moore Hall 569, University
of Hawaii at Manoa, 1890 East-West Road, Honolulu, HI 96822
Allan R. Bomhard :: 88-B Queen Street, Charleston, SC 29401-2427
Ron Christensen :: Institute for Medical Risk Studies, South Great
Road, Lincoln, Mass. 01773
Eric de Grolier :: 3 Rue St. Pierre, 45320 Courtenay, FRANCE. C.
John DiCara :: 9138 Mt. San Berdu Drive, El Paso, TX 79924
Merlin Donald :: Dep't. of Psychology, Queens University, Kingston,
Ontario K7L 3N6, CANADA.
Sheila Embleton : Dep't. of Langs, Lits, and Linguistics, York University,
4700 Keele Street, North York, Ontario M3J 1P3, CANADA
Harold C. Fleming :: 16 Butman Avenue, Gloucester, MA 01930-1006
Frederick C. Gamst :: 73 Forest Avenue, Cohasset, MA 02025
Joseph Harold Greenberg :: 860 Mayfield Avenue, Stanford, CA 94305
Kenneth Hale :: Room 20E-225, Massachusetts Institute of Technology,
Cambridge, Mass. 02139
Jerold S. Harmatz :: 935 Washington Street, #10, Newtonville, MA
02160-1562
LaVaughn H. Hayes :: 2021 Biltmore Drive, Fayetteville, NC 28304
Gy. Hegedüs Iren :: Janus Pannonius University, Dep't. of English,
Pécs, Ifjúság útja 6, H-7624 HUNGARY (Tel.
36-72-326-342) (FAX 36-72-326714) E-mail < ihegedus@btk.jpte.hu >
Gordon W. Hewes :: 335 16th St., Boulder, CO 80302 (or Dep't. of
Anthropology, University of Colorado, Boulder, CO 80309)
Carleton T. Hodge :: 3291 Spring Branch Road, Bloomington, IN 47401
Grover Hudson :: 714 Chittenden Drive, East Lansing, MI 48823
Stephen Huffman :: 1630 Hickory Knoll Road, Sandy Spring, MD 20860
John Hutchison :: 511 Main Street, West Newbury, MA 01985
Dell Hymes :: 205 Montvue Drive, Charlottesville, VA 22901
Herrmann Jungraithmayr :: Institut für Afrikanische Sprachwissenschaften,
Johan-Wolfgang-Goethe Universität, Kettenweg 135, D-60054 Frankfurt
a.M., GERMANY
E. Morgan Kelley, Jr. :: Dep't. of Modern Languages and Literatures,
The College of William and Mary, Williamsburg, VA 23185
Roland Kiessling :: Seminar für Afrikanische Sprachen und Kulturen,
Universität Hamburg, Rothenbaumchausee 67/69, 20148 Hamburg, GERMANY
Sydney W. Lamb :: 2419 McClendon, Houston, TX 77030
Bernd Lambert :: Dep't. of Anthropology, McGraw Hall, Cornell University,
Ithaca, New York 14853. E-mail < bl16@cornell.edu >
Joseph J. Lauer :: 1013 Beech Street East Lansing, MI 48823 (Africana
Bibliographer, Michigan State University Libraries, W313 Main Library,
East Lansing, MI 48824-1048)
Larry Lepionka :: University of South Carolina @ Salkahatchie, 1405
Newcastle Street, Beaufort, South Carolina 29902
Saul Levin :: 517 Harvard Street, Vestal, NY 13850
Philip Lieberman :: 141 Elton Street, Providence, RI 02906
Jean Lydall :: Zum Hainteich 27, 49326 Melle, GERMANY
Victor H. Mair :: 23 Oberlin Avenue, Swarthmore, PA 19081 James
P. Mallory :: Dep't. of Archeology, The Queen's University of Belfast,
Belfast BT7 1NN, Northern Ireland, UK
Daniel McCall :: 7 Wigglesworth Street, Boston, Mass. 02120
Peter A. Michalove :: 307 South McKinley Avenue, Champaign, IL 61821-3247
Duncan Murray :: 10049 East Bogue, Temple City, CA 91780
A.E. Murtonen :: 7 Essex Street, Footscray, Victoria 3011, Australia
Peter Norquest :: 1140 N. Roosevelt St., Boise, ID 83706-2542
Derek Nurse :: Linguistics Department, Memorial University of Newfoundland,
St. Johns, Newfoundland, CANADA A1B 3X9
Geoff N. O'Grady :: 1604 Mileva Lane, Victoria, British Columbia,
CANADA V8N 2V5 (Tel. 604-477-0731)
J. Joseph Pia :: 80 Alameda Street, Rochester, New York 14613-1419
Carroll L. Riley :: 1106 6th Street, Las Vegas, NM 87701
Merritt Ruhlen :: 4335 Cesano Court, Palo Alto, California 94306
Joseph C. Salmons :: German Dep't., 818 Van Hise, 1220 Linden Drive,
University of Wisconsin, Madison, WI 53706 E-mail < jsalmons@facstaff.wisc.edu
>
John Saul :: Oryx, 3 rue Bourdaloue, 75009 Paris, FRANCE
Wolfgang Schenkel :: Universität Tübingen, Ägyptologisches
Institut, In den Kreuzäckern 14, D-72072 Tübingen, GERMANY
W. Wilfried Schuhmacher :: Kirkebakken 13, 4621 Gadstrup, DENMARK
Stephen Sherry :: Dep't. of Anthropology, The Pennsylvania State
University, 409 Carpenter Building, University Park, PA 16802
Josephine Silvestro :: 57 Stanley Avenue, Medford, MA 02155 2116
Étienne Tiffou :: 3140 Kent Street, Montréal, Québec
H3S 1N1, CANADA (or Dep't. of Linguistics, Université de Montréal,
Québec, CANADA )
Anthony Traill :: Department of Linguistics, University of the Witwatersrand,
Johannesburg, 1 Jan Smuts Avenue, Johannesburg, SOUTH AFRICA (Tel. (011)
716-1111 :: FAX (011) 403-1926)
Larry Trask :: School of Cognitive and Computing Sciences, University
of Sussex, Falmer, Brighton, BN1 9QH, ENGLAND, UK (Trask's E-mail <
larryt@cogs.susx.ac.uk >
Stephen Tyler :: Dept. of Anthropology, Rice University, P.O.Box
1892, Houston, Texas 77251 E-mail < styler@ruf.rice.edu >
Andreas R. Wesserle :: 4257 North 52nd Street, Milwaukee, WI 53216
Paul Whitehouse :: Flat 3, Angel House, Pentonville Road, London
N1 9HJ, ENGLAND, UK
- - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - -
- -
Informational Editorial:
Borrowing Some Wisdom from Kindred Sciences Diachronically oriented
researchers tend to come upon similar general problems and react to such
in remarkably similar ways. I mean here to exclude documentary history
and theoretical evolutionism. The first tends towards similarity but is
usually so engrossed in details as to lose the general picture, while the
latter as part of general theory in anthropology is much more interested
in explanatory factors of cultural evolution with a marked tendency towards
economic cum ecological determinants. In more general philosophy of science
terms both of these aim towards outlining the narrative and explaining
the narrative, differing markedly in preferences for one or the other.
Biology in general, paleontology and systematics in particular, as well
as paleoanthropology, much of archeology, and traditional historical linguistics
are more oriented towards the narratives of large areas, whilst making
some explanatory efforts, because the narrative is so critical to their
efforts. In a word we cannot generate explanatory hypotheses until we know
what happened. A corollary to that is -- until we know what the relationships
among organisms have been. It is a striking feature of both theoretical
evolutionism and the hypotheses about language origins that both basically
lack interest in the narratives, especially after the original great creative
epoch (in the case of the latter). Or to paraphrase their mind sets --
who cares what happened after the Garden of Eden? The creation is everything!
We prehistorians in a general sense can learn from our colleagues in neighboring
fields.
Two examples are given here to show this. First, in biology, Lynn
Margulis with Karlene V. Schwartz shows one of our problems clearly. In
her FIVE KINGDOMS: An Illustrated Guide to the Phyla of Life on Earth.
1982. p.163, discussing "ANIMALIA", she wrote: "For many years (and even
today), biologists divided the animals, protozoans and metazoans together,
into two large groups: the invertebrates, those without backbones, and
the vertebrates, those with. In fact, all animals except the Craniata,
a subphylum of Phylum Chordata, belong to the invertebrate group. This
invertebrate / vertebrate dichotomy amply represents the skewed perspective
we have as members of Phylum Chordata. Our pets, beasts of burden, sources
of food, leather, and bone -- that is, the animals closest to our size
and best known to us -- are members of our own phylum. We now realize that,
from a less species-centered point of view, characteristics other than
backbones are more basic and reflect much earlier evolutionary divergences."
(End of quote.) In fact the overwhelming pre-occupation of linguists with
European languages, and historical linguists with Indo-European, looks
to be the true example of a "species-centered point of view". It surely
hinders our conquest of human prehistory. The viewpoint has been labeled
"Euro-centric" by Ruhlen. The major narrative of that to which I-E belongs
-- I-E's evolutionary background -- cannot be achieved by focusing on the
reconstruction of PIE. No matter how elegant and satisfying that reconstruction
may be it contributes little to the whole narrative because PIE is still
only a twig on a giant bush, just as our vertebrates are only a small branch
of the Animalia bush. Or do we really want to maintain that I-E has no
relatives and thus an utterly unknown evolutionary past? I hope not!
Michael Day, one of my favorite writers on prehistory, discusses
nomenclature and taxonomy in his useful Guide to Fossil Man, 4th Ed. 1986,
pp.12-13. Two of his points are germane to our inquiry. First, he cites
nomenclature rules in biology. "The naming of a new taxon must follow the
rules of nomenclature as set out in the International Code of Zoological
Nomenclature (Code Zoologique), a code adopted by successive International
Congresses of Zoology and since 1973 by the Division of Zoology of the
International Union of Biological Sciences. "The internationally accepted
object of the code is to promote stability and universality in the scientific
names of animals, and to ensure that the name of each taxon is unique and
distinct. Duplications must be avoided and the international understanding
and acceptance of names is a prime objective of the Code. Priority is the
basic principle of zoological nomenclature. It follows that nomenclature,
the naming of a taxon, is not simply a matter of choice and taste; if the
rules of the Code are not followed the world of zoology will not accept
the new name for the good reason that it will inevitably cause confusion
and controversy. The history of paleoanthropology is littered with nomenclatural
solecisms not all of which are in the distant past." (End of quote)
The situation is so bad in linguistics that a custom has arisen
which says that each field worker may change the name of his language if
he wishes to or thinks the old one not quite right phonetically. The reader
is referred to the closest approximation to chaos we have, David Dalby's
Thesaurus of African Languages, 1989; herein almost anything goes -- there
are numerous words for (e.g.) Kikuyu and even normal print is forsaken
for a kind of handwritten text and ordinary phonetic symbols are replaced
by an innovative Africanist set. It is all terribly avant garde and guaranteed
to reflect accurately its internal taxonomy -- numerous twigs and branches
in search of trees.
On taxonomy Day is even more useful. Quoting now: "The original
Linnean binomial system was designed for modern forms whose distinctiveness
could be determined by their inability to breed with other species and
produce like and fertile offspring. In palaeontology this possibility is
denied to us yet the affinity of a modern hippopotamus to a fossil hippopotamus
that was living some thousands of years ago cannot be denied. The Linnean
system of classification based on similarity encourages natural classification,
yet small evolutionary changes make precise delineation between successive
fossil species difficult to determine. The concept of the palaeo-species,
or chrono-species, evolving through time, is an attempt to deal with the
problem of the application of the Linnean system to palaeontology."
"An alternative approach is that of Hennig (1966). In this system
taxonomic significance is attached to the appearance of new morpho-logical
features in an evolving lineage that may indicate a branch point on an
evolutionary radiation of taxa. Features shared with previous evolutionary
stages in a lineage are discounted as features of common inheritance. The
sharing of new features is regarded as significant evidence of close kinship.
The codification of these schemes allows diagrams, termed cladograms, to
indicate relationships that are in no sense 'family trees' since a cladogram
has no time axis. Cladistic or phylogenetic analysis can throw interesting
and significant new light on the subject of primate taxonomy, particularly
at the higher levels of taxonomic resolution; it is less effective when
dealing with genera, species and subspecies. It can also produce inconsistent
and confusing results when misused."
"Linnean taxonomy, based on similarity, and Hennigian taxonomy,
based on difference, are complementary approaches to the problems of understanding
the natural world; they each have a contribution to make, they each have
their difficulties. Neither one system nor the other is wholly right or
wholly wrong, only the dogmatic application of either will lead to false
conclusions; indeed, the literature has examples already." (End of quote).
The Hennig reference is to W. Hennig. 1966. Phylogenetic Systematics. Urbana:
University of Illinois.
All of our taxonomic endeavours partake of the Linnean and Hennigian,
it seems. Not just in historical linguistics but also in archeology and
biogenetics. Concepts like shared mutations and shared innovations are
particularly close to the Hennigian. Notions of continuity or genetic connection
in archeology and much ordinary linguistic classification are more Linnean.
Much of what is 'obvious' is Linnean. What is different is the one-sided
scorn for the Linnean among 'mainstream' linguists, who nevertheless accept
the lower level (Linnean) groupings virtually without question and stop
there. Such impeccable logic! Day's arguments suggest that for long range
taxonomy more Hennigian approaches will work better, specifically because
similarities do get fewer and the obvious is no longer so. Much depends,
of course, on how many languages are compared. Although it seems that historical
linguists are cognitively challenged in the area of numbers, still even
they can see that seeking similarities across 20 word lists is almost always
going to produce more of them than you can get between 2 lists.
We need to look briefly at the ostensible dismissal of common inheritance
above in the Linnean discussion. What Day is arguing is that features of
the common inheritance can be discounted -- as evidence for close relationships.
If one will recall Trask's critique of the Basque-Caucasic hypothesis,
he argued that a 'genitive in -n' for example was not evidence that Basque
was related to Caucasic because such a grammeme is fairly common in human
languages. But this is tricky reasoning since it is only closeness that
is denied by this argument. Bengtson was not arguing that the two languages
were close, but rather that they were related, i.e., they shared an inheritance.
All those other languages which shared the grammeme were probably related
too! 'Not close' does not equal or mean 'not related at all'.
Day's point might be exemplified by stating that bats, cats, rats,
and beavers share warm blood and suckle their young, yet only beavers share
the special teeth with rats. Cats do not, so they are unrelated? And the
flying bats? Well, since warm blood and mother's milk are quite common,
they cannot show that bats and cats are related, either to each other or
to rats or beavers? Those who believe in taxon called Mammalia will deny
this and point out that these common features of mammals were once innovative
among animals. So rats and beavers have a closer relationship but along
with cats and bats they belong to a large taxon, itself part of Craniata,
part of Animalia. Enough said.
- - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - -
MIKE DiBLASI REPORTS ON HIS EXCAVATIONS AT AKSUM, ETHIOPIA
Consistent with our policy of occasionally focusing on matters of
lesser time depth than Indo-European, we give you a short summary of recent
research at Aksum. The importance of this research needs no profound justification
-- it is simply interesting. As part of the slow but sure process of unveiling
the prehistory of this most crucial of world areas for general human prehistory,
probably also for Nostratic and Afrasian in particular, we're trying to
publish almost everything that contributes to that goal. Aksum in Tigray
is a critical interface between Semites and Cushites, Meroe and Aksum,
N-S and Afrasian, and the trade of the Nile Valley cum Red Sea cum South
Arabia cum Indian Ocean cum the Ethiopian interior.
We have published some of S.Brandt's, A.Brooks', and K.Bard/R.Fattovici's
work before. We also call attention to gifted younger colleagues, like
Mike DiBlasi Because of format demands, we present the Bibliography first
(!). Bard,K. and R. Fattovici. 1995. "The I.U.O./B.U. Excavations at Bieta
Giyorgis (Aksum): An Interim Report", NYAME AKUMA 44:25-27 DiBlasi, M.
1996. "Results of the Analysis of Archeological Pollen Samples from Beta
Giyorgis, Aksum", Report to the Center for Research and Conservation of
the Cultural Heritage, Addis Ababa, Ethiopia. Fattovici, R. 1990. "Remarks
on the Pre-Aksumite Period in Northern Ethiopia", JOURNAL OF ETHIOPIAN
STUDIES XXIII: 1-33 Michels, J.W. 1994. "Regional political organization
in the Axum-Yeha area during the Pre-Axumite and Axumite eras", in C. Lepage
(ed.) Études éthiopiennes I. Actes de la Xe Conférence
internationale des études éthiopiennes. Paris: La Société
françois pour les études éthiopiennes; pp.61-69.
*************************************************************************************************